Home |
|
|
|
Stress problems due to environmental changes in captive slender lorises (Loris tardigradus)
In slender lorises, even
slight
changes of their environment cause excitement. Stress after
transfer to
unfamiliar surroundings may lead to gastric ulcera, fatty
liver, inanition
and death. Reduced food consumption is the most reliable sign
of stress.
Behaviour may be misleading; some animals during severe stress
make an
unusually tame and quiet impression. Noise seems to be a
particularly dangerous
stressor. Susceptibility to environmental stress can be
reduced by habituation.
High cages with sufficient horizontal branches in the upper
part and some
hideaways are also helpful.
In the past, mortality of
slender
lorises in captivity was high; according to Bertram (1984), for
instance,
mean longevity of eight captive-bred slender lorises was one
year, and
Hill (1935) kept his animals (n = 50) alive "up to two years";
slender
lorises can reach an age of more than 14 years (some of our
wild-caught
animals). In our breeding project, some cooperating zoos had
severe losses
in quarantine. We came to the conclusion that keeping of slender
lorises,
with regard to diet and climate, is rather easy, but that
psychic stress
is highly dangerous for this excitable species.
Since Cannon (1935) and Selye
(1956),
the word "stress" is used for physiological responses to
endogenous or
exogenous agents, serving to maintain the dynamic steady state
of viability.
Changes, for instance, of heart rate, blood pressure and
secretion of endocrine
glands, prepare the body for reactions on stressors such as
cold, heat,
restraint, perception of threat or sexual stimuli. Consequently,
stress
is a normal state of life, not necessarily connected with
suffering, and
has positive effects on health and well-being as long as the
stressors
remain within certain species-specific limits. Selye, therefore,
used the
terms "eustress" for adaptation processes with desirable effects
and "distress"
for pathogenic reactions.
Behaviour
indicating
stress (see also figures showing behavioural
signs of stress and facial
expressions)
In lorises, movements under
normal
circumstances look flowing and easy, but in emotion-arousing
situations
become tense, very slow, hesitant or jerky. Freezing to
immobility, flight
from the source of disturbance and attempts to hide are frequent
even after
minor disturbance. When the animals are scared, they usually
flee upwards
until further ascent is stopped by the ceiling of the cage.
Sometimes they
remain there, hanging upside down, frozen in sometimes unusual
postures
or hiding their faces, for a considerable time; after severe
disturbance
some animals even slept in this position. In calm animals,
relaxed postures
are frequent, for instance during resting and comfort behaviour;
under
stress they are absent. In situations causing severe stress
animals may
give a normal and quiet impression. In three cases, when we
tried to catch
shy animals, they began to appear unusually tame and quiet, made
no attempts
to flee when touched, and then suddenly had a convulsive
seizure; others
became apathetic for several seconds after being caught.
Seizures occurred
in stressful or exciting situations and were triggered by an
additional
stimulus, for instance by a noise or a sudden change in light
intensity.
Most of the seizures had the shape of epileptic grand mal
(rhythmic convulsions,
foam and saliva dripping from the mouth), one resembled feigning
death,
a protective reaction which might be closely connected to
epileptic seizures
(Rabe 1970). Reduced food consumption turned out to be the most
reliable
measure of stress and disease, but may not be noticed when the
animal is
caged together with others.
In general the animals recovered
quickly from short periods of stress followed by stay in quiet
familiar
environment; no disease or death is known to have been caused by
such events.
Some animals, however, after handling or other apparenly
traumatic events
remained shy for years.
Causes of death in adult slender lorises in the two colonies at Ruhr-University and Lennestadt between 1980 and 1993
Practical recommendations for transfer of animals to other facilities
Prophylactic measures against stress before transportation to a new environment:
Measures in
the new
environment after transfer
After transfer to an unfamiliar
environment
lorises need a good protecion from any avoidable disturbance
(noise, visibly
moving larger objects or persons); the fronts of their cages
ought to be
covered for instance with blankets. Small holes for observation
apparently
cause little stress because the perceived stimulus does not look
large;
even stressed animals approached the holes and looked through
them into
the observer´s eye from short distance. Cages containing plenty
of
cover (artificial plants) help the animals to feel safe and
recover soon.
Dim light during daytime in the
first
days in an unfamiliar environments may be useful because the
animals feel
safer in darkness (according to Perret, 1982, in Microcebus
a reduction
of daylength had a protective effect against diseases caused by
stress).
At least some food ought to be offered in a protected, high
place the animals
are not too afraid to approach. Change of diet under stress
conditions
ought to be avoided as some animals already under usual
circumstances hesitate
to try new food and during stress may refuse eating. Moving
insects (locusts,
mealworms) may encourage shy animals to eat and seem to improve
their psychic
state.